No one is born gay

This image shows the coding region in a segmen...
This image shows the coding region in a segment of eukaryotic DNA. (Photo credit: Wikipedia)

It is reasonable to assume that sexual behavior--in any and all of its manifestation—is directed by the activity of many, possibly hundred of genes. But the exact and specific type of sexual expression is also influenced by countless environmental forces that interact with the genes in complex ways (Hubbard, 1997; Ridley, 2003).

Except for the rare physical abnormalities (such as Huntington’s Disease) at the present time, there is no evidence of a direct causative link between a single gene and complex psycho-social behavior such as sexual preference (Collins, 2006). This is not my opinion but read any book on behavior genetics or molecular biology and the authors will unanimously agree that, at present, there is no gay gene.

Lewontin et al. stated this explicitly:
“Up to the present time no one has ever been able to relate any aspect of human social behavior to any particular gene or set of genes, and no one has ever suggested an experimental plan for doing so. Thus, all statements about the genetic basis of human social traits are necessarily purely speculative, no matter how positive they seem to be” (p. 251).

Dr. Francis S. Collins (MD and PhD) head of The Human Genome Project has said:
There is an inescapable component of heritability to many human behavioral traits. For virtually none of them is heredity ever close to predictive…An area of particularly strong public interest is the genetic basis of homosexuality. Evidence [indicates] that sexual orientation is genetically influenced but not hardwired by DNA, and that whatever genes are involved represent predispositions, not predeterminations. (2006).

Stein (1999), a psychologist explains:
Genes in themselves cannot directly specify any behavior or psychological phenomenon. Instead,genes direct a particular pattern of RNA synthesis, which in turn may influence the development of psychological dispositions and the expression of behaviors. There are many intervening pathways between a gene and a disposition or a behavior, and even more intervening variables between a gene and a pattern that involves both thinking and behaviors…No one has presented evidence in support of such a simple and direct link between genes and sexual orientation.

Baker (2004) a behavioral psychologist agrees:
Most physical traits and conditions such as height, blood pressure, weight and digestive activity stem from many genes that vary in activity depending on environmental contexts. The same is true of all complex [psycho-social] behaviors. Each is affected by multiple genes interacting with multiple environmental influences…Unfortunately; many people have a different impression. They think that a gene controls a behavioral trait. This is genetic determinism, the belief that the development of an organism is determined solely by genetic factors. Genetic determinism is a false belief. It comes from misunderstandings of scientific research…The fact is that so far,
scientific research has not confirmed any one-to-one correspondence between a gene and a [complex] human behavior. Behavior results from the activity of multiple genes amidst the influence of multiple environmental factors (p. 17-18).

Two scientists, McInerney and Rothstein, who have worked on the Human Genome Project, caution us when interpreting research on genetic “causes” of behavior:

How do genes influence behavior? No single gene determines a particular behavior. Behaviors are complex traits involving multiple genes that are affected by a variety of other factors. This fact often gets overlooked in the media report hyping scientific breakthroughs on gene function, and unfortunately, this can be very misleading to the public” (2007).

Evolutionary anthropologist Sarah Blaffer Hardy adds:
Nature cannot be compartmentalized from nurture, yet something about human imaginations predisposed us to dichotomize the world that way…Complex behaviors like nurturing, especially when tied to even more complex emotions like “love,” are never either genetically predetermined or environmentally produced” (from Hrdy’s 2000 book “Mother Nature”, cited in Ridley, 2003, p. 246).

Matt Ridley (2003) concurs:
Genes are enablers, not constrainers. They create new possibilities for the organism; they do not reduce its options…The new possibilities are open to experience, not scripted in advance. Genes no more constrain human nature than extra programs constrain a computer…Genes, unlike gods, are conditional. They are exquisitely good at simple if-then logic: if in a certain environment, then develop in a certain way…Don’t be frightened of genes. They are not gods, they are cogs” (Ridley, p. 250).

In summary, there is no undisputed evidence that same-sex behavior is hard-wired in the brain. Genes are
complex strands of DNA that through the processes of transcription and translation, direct the synthesis of
amino acids into larger proteins that influence cell structure and functioning (How Genes Work, 2007;
Schwartz & Azar, 1981).

Complex social activities such as sexual behavior cannot be directly traced to the activity of a single gene (Parens, Chapman & Press, 2006; Peele, 1995; Rutter, 2006). Many uninformed people take a simplistic view of behavioral genetics: they believe that one gene controls and determines a specific behavior. This is true for a very few, abnormal physical conditions including Huntington’s disease, cystic fibrosis, PKU, and achondroplasia (Dwarfism). This fact has led some to believe that there is an alcoholic gene, a manic-depression gene, or a gay gene. However, “Genes do not act as master puppeteers within us.

They are chemical structures that control the production of proteins; thereby indirectly affecting behavior…Genes do not determine one’s destiny” (Plomin et al., 1990, p.13). “It is an oversimplification to say that any gene is ‘the gene for a trait’. Each gene simply specifies one of the proteins involved in the process [of gene-environmental interaction], notes Hubbard (Hubbard & Wald, 1999, p. 44).

Complex psycho-social behaviors such as sexual preference are not determined by a single gene, but by a
gene-environmental process involving possibly hundreds of genes acting through complex environmental
factors (Rutter, 2006). “The fact is that so far, scientific research has not confirmed any one-to-one correspondence between a gene and a [complex psycho-social] behavior. Behavior results from the activity of multiple genes amidst the influence of multiple environmental factors” (Baker, 204, p. 18).

If you are still unconvinced that scientists have not found the “gay gene,” let me share with you one final bit of knowledge about behavioral genetics. Suppose you could isolate a segment of DNA that you thought was related to homosexuality. You could then specify the exact, let’s say 183,000 base pairs, that make up this portion of DNA.

The “gene” would look something like this sequence: TA, GC, TA, TA, GC, CG, AT, AT, AT, GC, GC, CG, TA….. The letter A, T, G, and C stand for the four nucleotides that make up DNA: adenine, thymine, guanine, and cytosine. Now you get DNA samples of other gay men and examine this exact 183,000 base-pair sequence on the same chromosome. You compare the sequencing of TA, CG, GC, and AT along the whole length of the gay gene. Surprise: The sequences from 10 gay men don’t nearly match! Some portions are similar, but most are dissimilar.

But you don’t give up. You recruit the brothers of the original 10 gay men. You code their nucleotides at the same foci on the suspected gay gene. You find that the DNA sequences of the gay/non-gay brothers are more similar than that of the gay/gay men. In other words, the non-gay brothers’ DNA sequence is more similar to their gay brothers than is the sequencing of the gay men to each other! You are persistent so try once more.

You recruit another 20 gay men and code their DNA. Again, you find that there is more dissimilarity in the sequences of base pairs between the gay men than similarities. Undaunted, you write another grant and keep looking. Scientists have actually done what I have just described but with only few inherited diseases like emophilia (Lippman, 1991). Hubbard concluded that most scientists believe that “base sequences can vary a great deal without any change being apparent in the corresponding trait” (1999, p. 55; also see Gianelli, 1990). That’s an amazing fact and puts the crimp in anyone’s plan to discover the elusive gay gene.


The genetic theory of homosexuality rests on a foundation of three seminal studies in the early 1990’s--
which all have serious methodological, sampling, and interpretation problems. Simon LeVay (1991) dissected the brains of 19 gay men and supposedly 16 non-gay men and found, on average, a slighter smaller area of the hypothalamus (INAH-3) in the gay men. He then “suggests that sexual orientation has a biological substrate.” There were several major flaws with his research: (a) the sample was small, (b) the control group was inappropriate, (c) there is no evidence that the INAH-3 part of the brain had anything to do with sexual preference, (d) AIDS could have caused the brain differences, and (e) the study has never been replicated.

Michael Bailey and Richard Pillard (1991) concluded there must be a genetic cause to homosexuality because they found higher rates of homosexuality among identical than fraternal twins and even less concordance (similarity) among adopted siblings. These quantitative genetic studies have similar limitations.

  • First, the samples may be biased because researchers usually recruit a volunteer sample from gay publications and organizations.

  • Second, such studies require a large sample in order to make valid heritability estimates, and samples are usually small.

  • Third, environmental factors are usually not studied  so its effects are unaccounted for.

  • Forth, there are obvious interpretation problems because only about half of identical twins reared in the same family have a gay brother. If genes determined homosexuality then both brothers should be gay.

  • Fifth, other twin studies have not supported their claim of a strong genetic component to homosexuality (see Hershberger, 1997). Dean Hamer and his colleagues (1993) examined a small section of the X chromosome in the families of 40 gay men. In a complicated pedigree analysis, Hamer claims that homosexuality is transmitted through
  • the maternal side and is genetically linked to the X chromosome region known as Xq28. His conclusion has been criticized by several authors (Baron, 1993). If homosexuality were a simple Mendelian trait (like eye color) then Hamer should have found a higher incidence of homosexuality among brothers. There is no evidence that the Xq28 section of the chromosome has anything to do with sexual behavior!

Hamer did not assess this genetic marker on the heterosexual men in the sample to see if they possessed it also. Rice, Anderson, Risch and Ebers (1999) did a similar but larger study (N=52) and found no support for an X-linked gene underlying male homosexuality. Hamer’s study has never been replicated. Thus, none of these studies proves a direct causation between a gene and the complex psychosocial behavior of sexual preference (Dailey, 2003; Lasco, Jordan, Edgar, Petito & Byne, 2002). The more amazing point is that all of these men (LeVay, Bailey, and Hamer) readily admit they there research does not prove there is a gay gene and that environment is a major factor in homosexuality!

Even if biogenic factors have a weak, but indirect, affect on sexual orientation on some individuals, Valenstein (1998) explains:

Most recent claims that a gene has been discovered that causes alcoholism, schizophrenia, [or] homosexuality…have proven illusory… genes do not produce behavioral or mental states. Genes carry the instructions and template for producing and assembling amino acids and proteins into anatomical structures. Behavior and mental traits; however, are the product of an interaction between anatomical structure and experience…Even where there is compelling evidence that some behavioral or mental trait is influenced by genetic factors it is almost always a predisposition, not a certainty… a predisposition is not a cause. (p.140-141, 224).

Even advocates for a genetic explanation of homosexuality such as Szuchman (2002), concluded that the
scientific evidence for a biological cause of homosexual behavior is “remarkably flawed, such that no unbiased view for or against many of these factors [i.e., biogenetic causes] is possible…We still have no good evidence of biological influences on sexual preference or sexual orientation” (p. 212). Gay advocates Parker and DeCecco (1995) conceded that “research into possible biological bases of sexual preference has failed to produce any conclusive evidence” (p. 427).

Genes or hormones may make it more likely that a person will display certain temperament characteristics or atypical gender role behaviors. These outcomes may make the child more vulnerable to child trauma (e.g., sexual abuse, negative fathering, and rejection by same-sex peers). The child’s psycho-social sequelae may, in turn, set up conditions where the child is more likely to consider the homosexual option (Stein, 1999).

Bancroft (1990) has stated that biological factors that may influence sexual orientation “need to be understood as interacting with the effects of social and cognitive learning rather than having direct effects of their own” (p. 109). Thus, genetic factor may indirectly influence sexual orientation, but there is no evidence of a direct casual link between a gay gene and homosexual behavior.

You may like the analogy of gene-environment interaction provided by Robert Plomin et al. (1980):

A sailboat needs both sails [environment] and a hull [genes]. The ‘behavior’ of a sailboat (speed, turning ability and direction) depends on the design of its sails and the design of its hull. The aerodynamic shape of the sails, their number and size, and their positioning are important. The depth, width, length, and shape of the hull are also important. Obviously, for sailboats, there can be no behavior without both sails and hull, but this does not restrict us from asking about the independent contributions of sail design and hull design to the behavior of sailboats…Behavior requires both genes and environment (p. 359).

Yet, even this example is flawed. The missing factor in Plomin’s analogy is the “captain” at the helm of the ship. That captain is “agency,” “free will” and “choice”! Regardless of sails (environmental factors) and hull design (genetic predispositions), the captain can make moral decisions independent of both sails and hull. The captain may be constrained by genes and environment—but he is not absolutely determined by them. Agency intervenes, on many occasions, at various times and places, in the interplay between genes and environment.

The British poet William Ernest Henley (1849-1903), in his famous poem “Invictus” (Latin for unconquerable) penned this famous line:

“It matters not how strait the gate, how charged with punishment the scroll. I am the master of my fate: I am the captain of my soul.”

There is clear evidence that many men and women are at the helm controlling their sexual behavior and making choices regarding sexual preference. I believe that the hypothetical evidence for genetic determinism of homosexuality is overstated and overrated. Most people do not have the knowledge to understand the research and are simply hoodwinked by the pro-gay activists.

It is obvious to me, and to many others, that environmental factors play the major role in same-sex behavior, if this were not so how does one explain the thousands of men and women who have left homosexuality. Consider the recent example of Michael Glatze, founder of Young Gay American Magazine, film producer, pro-gay lecturer and author, and well-recognized leader in the gay movement. At age 14 he believed himself to be “gay,” but at age 30 he “seriously began to doubt” what he was doing (Moore, 2007).

He explains:
Knowing no one who I could approach with my questions and my doubts, I turned to God…It became clear to me that homosexuality prevents us from finding our true selves…I was leading a movement of sin and corruption…Now I know that homosexuality is lust and pornography wrapped into one. I’ll never let anybody try to convince me otherwise…Healing from the wounds caused by homosexuality is not easy—there’s little support. In my experience, coming out from under the influence of the homosexual mindset was the most liberating, beautiful and astonishing
things I’ve ever experienced…I believe that all people, intrinsically know the truth. I believe that is why Christianity scares people so much. It reminds them of their conscience, which we all possess. Conscience tell us right from wrong and is a guide by which we can grow and become stronger and freer human beings (in Moore, 2007, p.3-5).

In conclusion, I believe that the genetic evidence for homosexuality is just not there. It’s the values and politics of homosexuals and their supporters that is driving the gay gene agenda, not good science.


References

Baker, C. (2004). Behavioral genetics: An introduction to how genes and environments interact throught development to
shape differences in mood, personality, and intelligence. New York: The American Association for the Advancement of Science and the Hastings Center. Entire book available free online at: http://www.aaas.org/spp/bgenes/publications.shtml
Bailey, J. M., & Dawood, K. (1998). Behavioral genetics, sexual orientation, and the family. In C. Patterson and A.
D’Augelli, Lesbian, gay, and bisexual identities in families, pp. 3-21. New York: Oxford University Press.
Bailey, J. M., & Pillard, R. C. (1991). A genetic study of male sexual orientation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 1089-
1095.
Bancroft, J. (1990). Commentary: Biological contributions to sexual orientation. In D. McWhirter, S. A. Sanders, and J. M.
Reinisch (Eds.), Homosexuality heterosexuality concepts of sexual orientation, pp. 101-111. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Banks, A., & Gartrell, N. K. (1995). Hormones and sexual orientations: A questionable link. Journal of Homosexuality, 28,
247-268.
Byne, W. (1995). Science and belief: Psychobiological research on sexual orientation. Journal of Homosexuality, 28, 303,
240.
Byne W., & Parsons, B. (1993). Human sexual orientation: The biological theories reappraised. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 50, 228-239.
Cartwright, S. Report on the diseases and physical peculiarities of the Negro race. The New Orleans Medical and Surgical Journal, May, pp. 691-715.
Collins, Francis S. (2006). The language of god, a scientist presents evidence for belief. New York: Free Press.
Gianelli, F. (1990). Haemophilia B: Data base of point mutations and short additions and deletions. Nucleic Acid Research, 18, 4053-4059.
Hamer, D. H., Hu, S., Magnuson, V. L., Hu, N., & Pattatucci, A. (1993). A linkage between DNA markers on the X
chromosome and male sexual orientations. Science, 261, 321-327.
Hershberger, S. L. (1997). A twin registry study of male and female sexual orientation. The Journal of Sex Research, 34,
212-218).
How genes work. (2007). Genetic Home Reference, A Handbook on How Genes Work. Retrieved online July 6, 2007 from: http://ghr.nlm.nih.gov/handbook/howgeneswork.pdf
Hubbard, R., & Wald, E. (1999). Exploding the gene myth. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.
Lasco, M., Jordan T, Edgar, M, Petito, C., & Byne, W. (2002). A lack of dimorphism of sex or sexual orientation in the
human anterior commissure. Brain Research, 936, 95-101.
Lewontin, R. C., Rose, S., & Kamin, L. (1984). Not in our genes. New York: Pantheon Books.
Le Vay, S. (1991). A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual and homosexual men. Science, 253, 1034-1037.
Lippman, A. (1991). Prenatal genetic testing and screening: Constructing need and reinforcing inequities. American Journal of Law and Medicine17, 15-50.
McInerney, J., & Rothstein, J. (2007). What is behavioral genetics? Retried online July 5, 2007 from:
http://www.ornl.gov.sci/techresources/Human_Genome/elsi/behavior.shtml
Moore, A. (2007). Gay-rights leader quits homosexuality. Posted on July 3, 2007 and available on-line at:
http://www.worldnetdaily.com.
Parens, E., Chapman, A, & Press, N. (2006). Wrestling with behavioral genetics: science, ethics, and public conversation.
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Parker, D. A., & DeCecco, J. P. (1995). Sexual expression: A global perspective. Journal of Homosexuality, 28, 427-430.
Pattatucci, A. M. (1998). Biopsychosicla interaction and the development of sexual orientation. In C. Patterson and A.
D’Augelli, Lesbian, gay, and bisexual identities in families, pp. 19-35. New York: Oxford University Press.
Pelle, S. (1995). My genes made me do it. Psychology Today, July/August, pp. 50-53 and 62-68.
Plomin, R., DeFries, J., & McClearn, G. (1980). Behavioral genetics: a primer. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman.
Rice, G., Anderson, C, Risch, N., & Ebers, G. (1999). Male homosexuality: Absence of linkage to micro- satellite markers at Xq28. Science, 284, 663-671.
Ridley, M. (2003). Nature via nurture: Genes, experience, and what makes us human. New York: HaperCollins
Rutter, M. (2006). Genes and behavior nature-nurture interplay explained. Ames, IO: Blackwell Publishing.
Satinover, J. (1996). Homosexuality and the politics of truth. Grand Rapids, MI: Baker Books.
Stein, E. (1999). The mismeasure of desire: The science, theory, and ethics of sexual orientation. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Schmidt, T. (1995). Straight and narrow? Downer Grove, IL: InterVarsity Press.
Schwartz, M., & Azar, M. (1981). Advanced cell biology. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Szuchman, L.T., & Muscarella, F. (2002). Psychological perspectives on human sexuality. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Valenstein, E. S. (1998). Blaming the brain: The truth about drugs and mental health. New York, NY: The Free Press

Enhanced by Zemanta

Popular posts from this blog

Ontario Catholic school board to vote on flying gay ‘pride flag’ at all board-run schools

Christian baker must make ‘wedding’ bakes for gay couples, court rules

Australia: Gay Hate tribunals are coming